Do you need to drop a few pounds? What color are your dishes? The UK's Daily Telegraph recently reported on a study published in the journal Appetite Researchers have reported that eating food off red-colored plates and drinking from red cups can cut consumption by approximately 40 percent. This may be because "red" is often associated with stop signals and danger (as in red fire engines). Red dishes may trigger stop associations in the brain. Researchers initially took 41 male students, and asked them to drink tea from cups with both red and blue labels. The participants drank 44 percent less from the red-labeled vessels. Part two of the study involved 109 people, who were instructed to eat pretzels laid out on either a red, blue, or white plate. Again, participants who were given the red plates reportedly consumed less. Some have suggested that using red cups in pubs and bars might help limit patrons’ alcohol consumption, and that using red packaging in supermarkets might serve as a deterrent for purchasing unhealthy foods.
http://forum.lowcarber.org/showthread.php?t=437612
Thursday, July 31, 2014
Wednesday, July 30, 2014
Brains and Pedophilic Behaviors, 2
Results of a study related to the
brains of people who exhibit pedophilic behaviors were recently released. The
research was led by Jorge Ponseti at the University of Kiel in Germany. His
team analyzed the MRI scans of 56 male participants, including 13 homosexual
paedophiles and 11 heterosexual paedohiles, while they were exposed to ‘highly
arousing’ images of men, women, boys, and girls. Participants were asked to
rate the images in terms of attractiveness. Dr. James Cantor, associate
professor at the University of Toronto’s Faculty of Medicine and expert on the
subject pedophilia told The Daily
Beast, “I have previously described pedophilia as a ‘cross-wiring’
of sexual and nurturing instincts, and this data neatly verifies that
interpretation.” This study may pave the way for future work into how this
knowledge can be used to test for, and hopefully rehabilitate, paedophiles
before they abuse. Part 3 tomorrow.
Tuesday, July 29, 2014
Brain and Pedophilic Behaviors, 1
Is the brain of someone who exhibits
pedophilic behaviors different from the brains of those who do not? Results of
a research project let by Jorge Ponseti at the University of Kiel in Germany, was
recently published in the scientific journal Biology Letters. Typically,
seeing the faces of children elicit feelings of caregiving from both males and
females. Ponseti’s studies showed that this trend appears to be skewed in the
brains of pedophiles. Their brains appear to be
“abnormally tuned” to find young children sexually attractive, children’s
faces eliciting sexual feelings as opposed to nurturing feelings. Is it
possible that the brains of pedophiles are wired to find children sexually
attractive rather than adults? Charlotte
Lytton reported in The Daily Beast that
the brains of “paedophiles react to images they find attractive in the same way
as most people—but for some reason this reaction happens when they see images
of children, rather than other adults.” Their perceptions appear to be turned
upside down. Part 2 tomorrow
Monday, July 28, 2014
Memory and Belly Fat
How's your memory? How much belly fat do you have? Researchers at
Rush University have reported that individuals who have high amounts of belly fat are more than three times as
likely to develop memory loss and dementia later in life. t's linked to the liver's hankering
for a protein that's also relished by the brain. The study, which
appeared in the journal Cell
Reports, reported that the liver burns belly fat with the help of a
protein known as PPARalpha. Who knew? It turns out that the brain uses this
same protein for memory. The liver works extra hard in individuals who have a
large amount of belly fat, using up the PPARalpha. And if the liver doesn't
have enough PPARalpha around it to use, it turns to other parts of the body to
find more--the brain! The hippocampus, the brain's search engine, which plays a
role in memory and learning, is essentially starved of PPARalpha. More research
is needed to find a way to maintain normal PPARalpha levels in the brain to
potentially prevent memory loss. In the meantime, exercise and eat healthy to
keep belly fat in check and stay sharp mentally.
Friday, July 25, 2014
Bias vs Prejudice, 6
Enter something called implicit
prejudice. It can be defined as prejudice that arises far below one’s level
of consciousness; although not expressed it is capable of being understood from something
else. According
to an article by Chris de Morsella entitled “Implicit Prejudice Unconsciously
Colors Our World, Implicit prejudice is a real and
still poorly addressed problem in the workplace and society. He writes, “While
the incidence of overt explicit prejudice and racism has plummeted in American
society over the last decades, implicit prejudice, which is prejudice that is
harbored subconsciously and is expressed inadvertently, is still widespread.
Unconsciously arrived at attitudes towards race, ethnicity, gender, sexual
orientation, disability etc. have a profound impact on the conscious opinions
we form and attitudes we adopt towards other individuals we encounter within
our work and social lives.” Studies have shown that human beings can only deal
effectively with what they can bring to conscious awareness, label, and
describe. Therefore, it might be beneficial if every human being spent some
time trying to bring implicit prejudices to conscious awareness. It reminds me of the words from the
old song: “When will we ever learn? When
will we ever learn?”
Thursday, July 24, 2014
Bias vs Prejudice, 5
Prejudice
plays a part in many areas of life. A study by a Vanderbilt University
professor of law and economics found legal immigrants in the United States with
a lighter skin tone made more money than those with darker skin. Researcher Joni
Hersch used data from 2,084 men and women who participated in the 2003 New
Immigrant Survey. An interviewer reported the person’s skin color using an
11-point scale where 0 represented the absence of color and 10 represented the
darkest possible skin color. Even when taking into consideration
characteristics that might affect wages (e.g., English language proficiency,
work experience and education), Hersch found immigrants with the lightest skin
color earned, on average, 8 percent to 15 percent more than immigrants with the
darkest skin tone. The effect of skin color even persisted among workers with
the same ethnicity, race, and country of origin. After I considering a whole
series of alternative interpretations and explanations, Hersh was both
surprised and dismayed at how strong and persistent the skin-color effect was.
She also found height played a part in salary. Taller immigrants earned more,
with every inch adding an additional one percent to wages. I am an immigrant
from Canada and barely five feet tall. And my skin color partly depends on how
much I’ve been out in the sun. . .
Hmm-m-m. Part 6 tomorrow.
Wednesday, July 23, 2014
Bias vs Prejudice, 4
I learned about “wild-turkey prejudice” while visiting
friends in the mountains of North Carolina. Out taking my morning
constitutional in this north-west corner of the State, I chanced to see a lone
wild turkey. (Some of you know my sensory preference is auditory, but I also
have an energy advantage in the right frontal lobe and tend to notice the
unusual.) The color of this bird’s head and neck feathers were an unusual shade
of vanilla-white. She was all alone, out foraging for her breakfast. I walked
around looking for other wild turkeys as I was accustomed to seeing them in a
group or flock of some size. Nada, zip. Back at the house I was told that this
wild turkey was an outcast. My friends had named her Hagar. Turns out that
birds are quite visual and Hagar’s head and neck plumage was different from all
the other birds. Because of this (I assume here), she had been ousted from the
flock and relegated to a solitary existence. It reminded me of a recent
conversation I’d had. After offering to speak at a specific four-year college,
I had been told very directly that I would never speak there because I was of a
different race from the majority of students. To say I was a bit dumbfounded
would be putting it mildly. I was tempted to say, “Our brains are all the same
color. What difference does race make?” I held my tongue but shook my head in disbelief. Part 5
tomorrow.
Tuesday, July 22, 2014
Bias vs Prejudice, 3
Every human
being is likely prejudiced about something in some way because every family
script will have some prejudice in it. People can even be prejudiced against others
they think are prejudiced. Research in the 1970’s began to show that prejudice tends
to be based on favoritism towards one’s own groups, rather than negative
feelings towards another group. According to Marilyn Brewer, prejudice
"may develop not because outgroups are hated, but because positive
emotions such as admiration, sympathy, and trust are reserved for the ingroup."
[Brewer, Marilynn B. (1999). "The Psychology of
Prejudice: Ingroup Love and Outgroup Hate?" Journal of Social Issues
55 (3): 429–44. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00126.}
And apparently, prejudice is not unique among the human species only. Wild
turkeys do it, too. At least the band of wild turkeys in North Carolina, do. Part
4 tomorrow.
Monday, July 21, 2014
Bias vs Prejudice, 2
Prejudice can
be defined in several differing ways.
- A prejudgment
before becoming aware of the relevant facts; feeling, favorable or
unfavorable, toward a person or thing, prior to, or not based on, actual
experience.
- A preconceived
opinion or judgment or attitude, usually unfavorable (and sometimes
hostile), toward a person or group because of gender, social class,
financial situation, gender, political opinion, age, disability, religion
(or fractions with a religion), sexuality, race, ethnicity, culture,
language, nationality, or any other supposed characteristic or belief.
As separate
from bias, prejudice appears to be learned. That “learning” can be passed along
in the script each human being is handed (at least metaphorically) at birth and
probably passed through cellular memory, as well. It is likely strengthened by
what the person observes in his or her environment, including how the
individual is treated. Part 3 tomorrow.
Friday, July 18, 2014
Bias vs Prejudice, 1
Speaking of conflict, the word bias can be
defined as a mental tendency or inclination, or a preference that inhibits
impartial judgment. Studies suggest that the human brain has a built-in bias
for being more comfortable around whatever is familiar, like it. Some say the
fastest judgment a brain ever makes when it first sees someone or something is:
“This is like me, this is familiar,” or “This is not like me, this is
unfamiliar.” When I meet another human being for the first time, my brain
instantly catalogs a myriad of comparisons, whether that person is:
·
Like me (female) or
different (male)
·
Short like me or tall
·
Of European ancestry
like me or not
·
Wearing similar clothing to me or
not
·
And so on and so on…
Prejudice is a separate concept from bias. Part
2 next week.
Thursday, July 17, 2014
DNA Ancestry, 7
Based
on DNA testing, my patrilineal ancestry reportedly goes back to one African
male known as “Y-Chromosomal Adam.” Two initial descendants from this male were
the Haplogroup A and the Haplogroup BR. Other mutations occurred that were
split into Haplogroup F, Haplogroup P, and eventually Haplogroup R (distinguishable
by its M207 mutation). And the Y-chromosome results are aligned with a section
of this group known as Haplogroup R1b, reportedly related to a man in Iberia
(modern day Spain) now known as ‘the Patriarch, who carried the genetic marker
that designates the Haplogroup R1b. My cousin Tim is seeing what he can find
about my father’s generational line, because it appears they came to Canada from
either Ireland (100% of the male population in Western Ireland belongs to Haplogroup
R1b) or from England (70% of the male population in southern Britain belongs to
Haplogroup R1b). Anyway, if we could go back far enough, it appears that all of
us are related to all of us. Interesting concept, especially in light of all the existing conflicts, locally and globally.
Wednesday, July 16, 2014
DNA Ancestry, 6
I
told my one-and-only brother about my venture into DNA testing for maternal mitochondria,
and he graciously agreed to send in some of his white blood cells so we could
find out more about the Y-chromosomes of our ancestors. On my father’s side,
the markers show alignment with Haplogroup R1b, a Western European lineage that
is now the most prevalent Haplogroup worldwide. Although the Y-chromosome is
much smaller than the X-Chromosome, there are more markers from the Y-chromosome
ancestry test. Go figure! I’ll list those number below, too. Part 7 tomorrow.
Region
|
Marker
|
DYS 391
|
11
|
DYS 3891
|
12
|
DYS 439
|
11
|
DYS 38911
|
28
|
DYS 438
|
16
|
DYS 437
|
15
|
DYS 19
|
14
|
DYS 392
|
13
|
DYS 393
|
13
|
DYS 390
|
23
|
DYS 385
|
12,14
|
Tuesday, July 15, 2014
DNA Ancestry, 5
According
to DNASolutions Pty. Ltd., my common female ancestor is known as Mitochondrial
Eve. Four initial groups of descendants known as Haplogroups Lo-L3 are related
to Mitochondrial Eve. Group Lo apparently is now extinct, but Group L-3 divided
into two subtypes: M and N. A DNA marker at position 10875T of my mitochondrial
DNA, shows that I am a descendent of Haplogroup
N. Of course, there were more branchings and a woman classified as “Helena”
(meaning light in Greek) marked the beginning of my mitochondrial type: Haplogroup
H. One of the most famous of my Haplogroup H ancestors (that can be traced back
to Bertha Von Putelendorf who died 1190) is reported to be the French queen
Marie Antoinette—who, unfortunately, “lost her head.” Because that family was
quite prolific, some others in Haplogroup H include Marie-Louise of Austria
(Napoleon’s wife), the Empress Alexandra Fyodorovna (wife of the last Russian
tsar Nicolas II), and Britain’s Queen Victoria. Part 6 tomorrow.
Monday, July 14, 2014
DNA Ancestry, 4
Unlike
chromosomal DNA that is inherited from both parents, you get all your
mitochondrial DNA from your mother. Mutations accumulate in mitochondrial DNA
more quickly than in chromosomal DNA, so it's possible to trace your maternal
ancestry way back beyond any relatives you may know by name—simply by tracking
the inheritance of mutations in mitochondrial DNA. Back to where I started a
few days ago: I decided
to bite the bullet and send in some of my white blood cells to be analyzed for
mitochondrial DNA. In due time the results came back. Apparently the markers
that DNA Solutions identified show I am part of “Haplogroup H,” the most common
Haplogroup in Europe, occurring in 40%-60^ of the population. I’ll list my
numbers below just in case I may be related to some of you. Part 5 tomorrow.
Marker
|
16519C
|
152C
|
263G
|
315.1C
|
Friday, July 11, 2014
DNA Ancestry, 3
A mutation is a change in the spelling of a DNA
sequence (think of your body having a spell-check for DNA sequences and that
for some reason or other, it fails). Your DNA contains mutations that typically
are quite harmless. Some, however, are harmful and may be responsible for
triggering abnormal conditions and specific diseases. For example, sickle cell
anemia can be caused by a change in one single gene! Although 99% of your DNA
is located in your chromosomes, the remaining
1% of your DNA is located in the mitochondria. The mitochondria in human cells are the energy factories that
produce the energy-rich molecule known as ATP or adenosine triphosphate. Scientists
are linking mitochondrial DNA defects with a wide range of age-related diseases
including neurodegenerative disorders, some forms of heart disease, diabetes, and
various cancers. Part 4 next week.
Thursday, July 10, 2014
DNA Ancestry, 2
According to Dr. John Stamatoyannopoulos, University
of Washington, associate professor of genome sciences and of medicine, for over
forty years it has been assumed that DNA changes affecting the genetic code
solely impacted how proteins were made—now it appears that this basic
assumption about reading the human genome missed half of the picture. New
findings highlight that DNA is an incredibly powerful information storage
device, which nature has fully exploited in unexpected ways. About 15% of the
64-letter (codon) alphabet are dual-use letters known as duons. They
simultaneously specify both amino acids and something called transcription
factor (TF) sequences. This means that many DNA changes that appear to alter
protein sequences may actually cause disease by disrupting gene control
programs or even both mechanisms simultaneously. Part 3 tomorrow.
Wednesday, July 9, 2014
DNA Ancestry, 1
One fine day (after watching a TV program about descendants of President Thomas Jefferson) I decided to bite the bullet and send in some of my white blood cells to be
analyzed for maternal mitochondrial DNA. Before I go into that, a bit of background. As
you probably already know, your complete
set of genetic information is encoded within 23 pairs of chromosomes in the nucleus
of your cells. (Not all cells have a nucleus, however. Red-blood cells, for instance,
have no nucleus.) A chromosome is a single piece of coiled DNA, a bio-molecule that holds the blueprint for
how living organisms are built. About 99% of all DNA (deoxyribonucleic acid) in your body is found in your chromosomes. DNA
consists of a 64-letter (codon) alphabet that spells out your genetic code. The
letters or codons are organized into words and sentences called genes―a
segment of DNA passed down from parents to child that confers a trait to the
offspring. Humans have 25,000-30,000 genes, usually in pairs (one from each
parent). Part 2 tomorrow.
Tuesday, July 8, 2014
Blink of an Eye, 2
A study by MIT neuroscientists provides
evidence that feedforward processing—the
flow of information in only one direction, from retina through visual processing
centers in the brain—is sufficient for the brain to identify concepts without
having to do any further feedback processing. It also suggests that while the
images are seen for only thirteen milliseconds before the next image appears,
part of the brain continues to process those images for longer than that, senior
author Mary Potter explained, because in some cases subjects weren’t asked
whether a specified image was present until after they had seen the sequence. “If
images were wiped out after 13 milliseconds, people would never be able to
respond positively after the sequence. There has to be something in the brain
that has maintained that information at least that long,” she says. This
ability to identify images seen so briefly may help the brain as it decides
where to focus the eyes, which dart from point to point in brief movements
called fixations about three times per second. Deciding where to move the eyes
can take 100 to 140 milliseconds, so very high-speed understanding must occur
before that.
Monday, July 7, 2014
Blink of an Eye
It’s hard to keep up with the rapid
advances of brain research. Funded by the National Institutes of Health, MIT
neuroscientists found that the human brain can process entire images that the
eye sees for as little as 13 milliseconds—the first evidence of such rapid
processing speed—which works out to seventy-five frames per second. This is far
faster than the 100 milliseconds suggested by previous studies. In the new study, which appears in the journal Attention, Perception, and
Psychophysics, researchers asked subjects to look for a particular type of
image, such as “picnic” or “smiling couple,” as they viewed a series of six or
twelve images, each presented for between thirteen and eighty milliseconds. “The fact that
you can do that at these high speeds indicates to us that what vision does is
find concepts. That’s what the brain is doing all day long — trying to
understand what we’re looking at,” says Mary Potter, an MIT professor of brain
and cognitive sciences and senior author of the study.
Friday, July 4, 2014
Happy National Day
According to Wikipedia, the National Day is a designated date on which celebrations mark the nationhood of a non-sovereign country. This nationhood can be symbolized by the date of becoming a republic, of becoming independent, or a significant date for a patron saint or a ruler. The day is not always called by the name National Day but can be considered as one. Often it is also a national holiday. Some countries have more than one National Day. Pakistan has three, for example, none of which is named the National Day. In the United States of America, its Independence Day, commonly known as the Fourth of July, is the country's National Day and is a federal holiday. It commemorates the approval and adoption of the Declaration of Independence on July 4, 1776, when the then colonies declared independence from the United Kingdom. In a remarkable coincidence, both John Adams and Thomas Jefferson, the only signers of the Declaration of Independence later to serve as US Presidents, died on the same day: July 4, 1826, which was the 50th anniversary of the Declaration. Whatever your plans, have a happy day and relish your freedom.
Thursday, July 3, 2014
Casual Pot Use and Brain Changes, 3
Study results (recently published in the Journal of Neuroscience
related to recreational use of marijuana and brain changes in the amygdala and nucleus
accumbens) fit with animal studies that show when rats are given
tetrahydrocannabinol (THC)—the mind-altering ingredient contained in marijuana—their
brains rewire to form many new connections. In animals, these new connections
indicate the brain is adapting to the unnatural level of reward and stimulation
from marijuana. The result of these new connections is to make other natural
rewards less satisfying. Lead author Jodi Gilman said, “It may be that we’re
seeing a type of drug learning in the brain. “We think when people are in the
process of becoming addicted, their brains form these new connections.”
Wednesday, July 2, 2014
Casual Pot Use and Brain Changes, 2
An article was published recently in the Journal of Neuroscience
related to abnormal changes in the brain associated with the recreational use
of marijuana. The degree of brain abnormalities in these regions is directly
related to the number of joints a person smoked per week. The more joints a
person smoked, the more abnormal the shape, volume and density of the brain
regions. According to Dr. Breiter, this study raises a strong challenge to the
idea that casual marijuana use isn’t associated with negative consequences.
Some of the participants “used marijuana to get high once or twice a week. People
think a little recreational use shouldn’t cause a problem if someone is doing
okay with work or school. Our data directly says this is not the case.” Part 3
tomorrow.
Tuesday, July 1, 2014
Casual Pot Use and Brain Changes
The results of research led by Hans
Breiter, MD, professor of Psychiatry and Behavioral
Sciences at Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine and a
psychiatrist at Northwestern Memorial Hospital, was recently published in the Journal of Neuroscience. Lead
author Jodi Gilman, a researcher in the Massachusetts General Center for
Addiction Medicine and an instructor in Psychology at Harvard Medical School, indicated
this is the first study to show that the casual use of marijuana is related to
major brain changes in two areas: the nucleus accumbens and the amygdala. Both
these regions play a major role in emotion and motivation. Co-senior study
author Anne Blood, director of the Mood and Motor Control Laboratory at
Massachusetts General and assistant professor of psychiatry at Harvard Medical
School, said that these brain areas form the basis for how you assess positive
and negative features about things in the environment and make decisions about
them. Part 2 tomorrow.
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)