Try this, take a piece of
paper and draw a dot on it. Count backwards from ten to zero. When you start to
say the word zero put your finger on the dot and press it as if it were a
button. Pay attention and ask yourself: “At what moment did my brain tell my
finger to move?” In one sense, your brain—although it is inside your skull and
part of your brain-body—can be thought of as a separate entity. The brain can
only do what it thinks it can do and you are the one who tells it what it can
do. That forms part of the basis for learning to talk to you brain as if it
were a separate entity and you are giving it directions about what it can do.
So you say, “Maylene, you exercise for 20 minutes every morning before
breakfast.” “Jacob, you drink a glass of water before every meal.” That helps
‘to program’ your brain and tell it what you are expecting—and now the two of
you work together to accomplish what you want to have happen.
Tuesday, February 28, 2017
Monday, February 27, 2017
Who Makes the Decision?
Did you know that taking
some physical action—reaching for a glass of water, for example—appears to
begin in one part of your subconscious brain even before your brain consciously
‘wills’ to do the action and initiates
it? Researchers such as Hans Helmut Kornhuber and Luder Deecke, as well as
Benjamin Libet in the 1980s have studied this phenomenon using simple finger
movement. Kornhuber and Deecke found a precursor spike in the brain 0.8 seconds
before the finger actually moved and dubbed it the ‘readiness potential.’ They
also found a second and smaller spark of electrical activity at 0.05 seconds
before actual finger movement. Libet used a clock to identify the moment of
‘wanting to move a finger.’ That moment occurred after the point of readiness potential
but before sensors detected any actual finger movement. Conclusions were that
part of your brain ‘wills’ an action to occur before your conscious brain makes
you aware of your desire to make it happen. Hmmm.
Friday, February 24, 2017
Sayings #5
1. I like
long walks, especially when they're taken by people who annoy me.
2. I was
going to wear my camouflage shirt today, but I couldn't find it.
3. If at
first you don't succeed, skydiving is not for you.
4. Sometimes
I wake up grumpy; other times I let him sleep.
5. If
tomatoes are technically a fruit, is ketchup a smoothie?
6. Money is
the root of all wealth.
7. No matter
how much you push the envelope, it'll still be stationery.
8. I thought
my brain was really unique until someone told me that every brain is.
Thursday, February 23, 2017
Recognizing Faces, 7
The
difficulty in recognizing faces on your own-race effect verses other races
seems to be related to increased ability to extract information about the
spatial relationships between different facial features. Daniel T. Levin has
reportedly explained it this way: a deficit occurs when viewing people of
another race because visual information specifying race takes up mental
attention at the expense of individuating information when recognizing faces of
other races. It will be interesting if further research using perceptual tasks
can shed light on the specific cognitive processes involved in the other-race
effect. Studies in 2007 led by Bernstein, found that the own-race
effect likely extends beyond racial membership into concepts of in-group versus
out-group. For example, research showed that categorizing somebody by the
university he or she attends showed similar results compared to studies about
the own-race effect. Hugenberg, Miller, and Claypool (2007)
performed a study in which they introduced people to the concept of the
own-race effect before presenting them with a series of differing faces. If study
participants were made aware of the own-race effect prior to the experiment,
the study participants showed significantly less if any own-race effect. To me, this sounds like
‘knowledge is power,’ which can enable individuals to alter their perceptions
if they choose to do so.
Wednesday, February 22, 2017
Recognizing Faces, 6
In general,
humans find it easier to recognize faces within one’s own race. That is, humans
tend to perceive people of other races than their own to all look alike. As early as 1914 researchers were studying
differences in own-race recognition versus other-race recognition. Other things
being equal, individuals of a given race are distinguishable from each other in
proportion to one’s familiarity and contact with the race as whole. To the
uninitiated Caucasian, all Asians look alike; to the uninitiated Asian, most
native Americans or African Americans or Caucasians look alike. This
phenomenon is known by several names: cross-race effect, own-race effect,
other-race effect, own race bias, interracial-face-recognition-deficit, and so
on. This cross-race effect seems to appear around six months of age in human
beings. Interesting, researchers have found it can be altered in early
childhood through adulthood through interaction with people of other races. More tomorrow.
Tuesday, February 21, 2017
Recognizing Faces, 5
Specific mental challenges
appear to impact face and emotion perception. Take schizophrenia for example. When
asked to match faces, remember faces, and recognize which emotions are present
in a face, individuals with schizophrenia demonstrate worse accuracy and slower
response time. Schizophrenia patients are able to easily identify a happy
facial affect but struggle to identify faces as sad or fearful. Furthermore,
the severity of schizophrenia symptoms has been found to correlate with the
severity of impairment in face perception. Impairments in face and emotion
perception are linked to impairments in social skills, due to the individual's
inability to distinguish facial emotions Individuals with diagnoses of both schizophrenia
and antisocial personality disorder have been found to have even more
impairment in face and emotion perception (as compared with individuals with
just schizophrenia). These individuals have been found to struggle to identify
anger, surprise, and disgust. There is also a link between aggression and
emotion perception difficulties for people with this dual diagnosis. More
tomorrow.
Monday, February 20, 2017
Recognizing Faces, 4
It appears that early visual experience is
important to the development of face-processing skills. At birth it is thought
that a baby is able to see clearly about a foot in front of its eyes, while
actual visual recognition probably takes a few weeks. Study conclusions differ
in attempting to identify when a newborn can recognize its mother’s and
father’s face. Some research suggests babies may be able to recognize parental
faces within days of birth while other research estimates this could take up to
a couple month. (If you are discussing voice recognition, some researchers
believe newborns can recognize parental voices (they heard frequently during
gestation) almost immediately after birth. And in terms of recognizing scent,
breastfed newborns appear to become familiar with their mother's unique scent
very quickly. Visual recognition skills improve rapidly, however. By the time
an infant is about eight months old, he or she will likely be able to recognize
parents and familiar care providers from across a room. From across a crowded
room? Maybe not . . . More tomorrow.
Friday, February 17, 2017
Recognizing Faces, 3
Human
brains learn to discern among
the types of faces they see the most frequently. Studies at England's
University of Sheffield suggest that babies are born with a broad idea of what
a face is. They start out with the ability to recognize a
wide range of faces, even among races or species different from their own. Fast forward to the age of nine months,
however, and processing of faces narrows based on faces the babies see most
often. For example, if infants are exposed mainly to Asian faces, they will
gradually become less skilled at discerning among different faces of other
races. Study results suggest that broad exposure to races and
other species in infancy may prevent that loss of ability. The National
Geographic News reported on studies that focused on face processing—the ability to recognize and
categorize faces, determine identity and gender, and read emotions. Six-month-old study participants were able to recognize
the faces of individuals of a different races as well as different species
(e.g., monkey faces). Infants who received visual training for this retained
the ability. Infants with no training lost the skill by the time they were nine
months old. Their findings suggest
that, in humans, this skill is another instance of "use it or lose
it." More next week.
Thursday, February 16, 2017
Recognizing Faces, 2
Humans find it almost impossible to
recognize a face if it is upside down, or lit from an unfamiliar angle, or
viewed as a photographic negative. (Interestingly, individuals diagnosed with
schizophrenia are sometimes able to identify a face when seen upside down.) Some
studies have shown gender differences
in facial recognition. In general, men tend to recognize fewer faces of women
than women do, whereas no sex differences were found with regard to male faces.
When attempting the complex task of recognizing faces, nerve pathways make
connections in the brain to recall memories. A person’s voice can help with
facial recognition. The Seminal Model of face perception, proposes three stages
of face processing including recognition of the facial features, recall of
memories linked with that face, and name recall—which may or may not occur in
that order. The representation of an entire face may
be filed in a single neuron. This is sometimes referred to as the Jennifer
Aniston phenomenon. Researchers reportedly touched a single neuron inside a
person’s brain and the patient reported seeing Jennifer Aniston’s face. More tomorrow.
Wednesday, February 15, 2017
Recognizing Faces
Recognizing faces is one of the most difficult visual tasks
humans perform, because faces are so similar in composition. Scientists have discovered some interesting
things about how the brain recognizes faces. For example, the
brain appears to have a specialized mental module dedicated to face processing.
The fusiform gyrus in the temporal lobe appears
to be involved and at least partly responsible for one’s ability to recognize
faces. (Prosopagnosia is the term for damage to the temporal lobe that results
in an inability to recognize and identify familiar faces, even one’s own face.)
All faces have the same basic features and typically
consist of: two eyes, one nose, one mouth, a forehead, cheeks, chin, and
eyebrows, etc., but each person has some of its own distinguishing
features. Naturally, the more similar
two faces are (e.g., identical twins or mirror twins) the more difficult it is
to tell the difference between the two. How many faces can
your brain recognize? 10,000 or so on average. More tomorrow.
Tuesday, February 14, 2017
“Bath Salts” and the Brain, 2
Synthetic cathinone
products are often marketed as “bath salts.” They contain mind-altering
ingredients that can
cause a range of effects including: Synthetic cathinones can produce effects
that include: paranoia, hallucinations, increased sex drive, panic attacks,
excited delirium with extreme agitation and violent behavior, nosebleeds,
sweating, nausea, dehydration, breakdown of skeletal muscle tissue, and kidney
failure. Intoxication from synthetic cathinones has resulted in death. The
worst outcomes are associated with snorting or needle injection. And, yes, they
can be addictive. Animal studies show that rats will compulsively
self-administer synthetic cathinones. Human users have reported that the drugs
trigger intense cravings—uncontrollable urges to use the drug again. Taking
synthetic cathinones often may cause strong withdrawal symptoms that include:
depression, anxiety, tremors, problems sleeping paranoia. No medications are
currently available to treat addiction to synthetic cathinones. Naturally,
prevention is the best policy.
https://www.drugabuse.gov/publications/drugfacts/synthetic-cathinones-bath-salts
Monday, February 13, 2017
“Bath Salts” and the Brain
Epsom salts
that people use during bathing have no mind-altering ingredients. Synthetic
cathinone products marketed as "bath salts" do have mind-altering
ingredients and can be downright dangerous. These human-made drugs are
chemically related to cathinone, a stimulant found in Khat, a shrub grown in
East Africa and southern Arabia. People sometimes chew leaves for their mild
stimulant effects. One study found that 3,4-methylenedioxypyrovalerone (MDPV),
a common synthetic cathinone, affects the brain in a manner similar to cocaine
but is at least 10 times more powerful.( Molly—slang for "molecular,"
refers to the pure crystal powder form of MDMA). Usually purchased in capsules,
Molly has become more popular in the past few years. MDPV is the most common
synthetic cathinone found in the blood and urine of patients admitted to
emergency departments. These “bath salts” often take the form of a white or
brown crystal-like powder and are sold in small plastic or foil packages
labeled "not for human consumption" or as "plant food,"
"jewelry cleaner," or "phone screen cleaner." They may be
purchased via the internet and in some drug paraphernalia stores under a
variety of brand names, including: Flakka,
Bloom, Cloud Nine, Lunar Wave, Vanilla Sky, White Lightning, and Scarface. Typically “bath salts” are
swallowed, snorted, smoked, or injected. So what’s the effect of these
synthetic cathinones on the brain? More tomorrow.
Friday, February 10, 2017
Aphorisms
Do you enjoy aphorisms? I certainly do and have quite a collection. Spoken or written, aphorism literally means “definition.”
The term may have originated with Hippocrates,
the Greek physician regarded as the father of modern medicine. He used aphorismos
(a Greek ancestor of aphorism meaning
"definition") as the title of his book outlining his principles on
the diagnosis and treatment of disease. aphorisms can be defined as:
- A pithy observation that contains a general truth
- A statement of truth or opinion expressed in a
concise and witty manner
From time to time I shall share some aphorisms with you
from my collection. This is a favorite of mine:
- Better to be silent and be thought a fool than to
open one’s mouth and remove all doubt
Sayings #4
1. There may
be no excuse for laziness, but I'm still looking.
2. Women
spend more time wondering what men are thinking than men spend thinking.
3. Give me
ambiguity or give me something else.
4. He who
laughs last thinks slowest.
5. Is it
wrong that only one company makes the game Monopoly?
6. Women
sometimes make fools of men, but most guys are the do-it-yourself type.
7. I was
going to give him a nasty look, but he already had one.
8. Change is
inevitable, except from a vending machine.
9. The grass
may be greener on the other side of the fence but at least you don't have to
mow it.
Thursday, February 9, 2017
Water and Mental Clarity
Recently
I was visiting friends in another part of the country. Also visiting were a
pair of elderly twins, who were evidencing a great deal of confusion resembling
symptoms of early dementia. In the process of observing them, they appeared to
be drinking less than one glass of water a day (besides whatever they got from
their food) and there was no known contraindication to their reducing fluid
intake). We began encouraging them to drink more water. It was humorous
(almost) to put a glass of water in front of each of them and hear them say
they were not thirsty and did not need water. Knowing that thirst perception
does tend to diminish with age, we eventually got them on board by just sitting
down and drinking water with them. Within a day or two it was amazing to note
their confusion disappearing. They had more physical and mental energy and
their sense of humor was returning. Studies have shown that dehydration is
lethal for brain function because the brain (nearly three-quarters fluid) uses
water for its ‘thinking’ functions as well as for energy. Fortunately, becoming
more hydrated through regular water intake appears able to reverse some
symptoms of ‘senile dementia’ (as it was once called).
Wednesday, February 8, 2017
Rat Brain – Human Brain
“How is it that
rats are often used for studies and then someone thinks the conclusions can translate to
humans?” Good question. First, studies have shown that rat brains are
more like human brains than one might think. Neuroscientists
face a multitude of challenges trying to better understand the human brain. Because
of model organisms such as the rat, researchers are able to discover
information that might not otherwise be known because some experiments would be
impossible to do on humans. What are some of the similarities? For starters a
recent paper published in the journal Frontiers in Neural Circuits by Jared
Smith and Kevin Alloway indicated the discovery of a parallel between the motor
cortices of rats and humans that signifies a greater relevance of the rat model
to studies of the human brain than scientists had previously known. For another, rat and human brains have more than
30 identical peptides. Peptides are molecules consisting of two or more amino
acids that impact mood; some are hormones, others are neurotransmitters, and
some are a combination of both. Therefore, depending on the topic under
research, what happens in the rat brain may be very similar to what goes on in
the human brain. Maybe being called a “rat” isn’t so far off base after all . .
.
Tuesday, February 7, 2017
Plasticity or Not
Fortunately, the
human brain generally is quite flexible. Learning new words and information,
honing skills, or recognizing patterns, typically triggers your brain to grow
new dendrites on your neurons and/or modify and strengthen those already there.
Practicing the piano, or honing a sporting skill for example, helps pave these
neuron pathways. The brain rewires itself to accommodate the learning. That’s
called ‘plasticity.’ Researchers have discovered that the brain with dyslexia—the
most common learning challenge worldwide—may have difficulty rewiring itself with
learning, especially with reading as it is a very complex activity. Scans
showed that non-dyslexic brains showed less activity to words they had seen or
heard multiple times, the brain already adapting to process the information
more efficiently. The dyslexic brain appears to fully reprocess the stimuli
with each exposure rather than rewiring neural shortcuts. Now the question will
be if researchers can figure out how to assist the dyslexic brain with
increasing its plasticity.
Monday, February 6, 2017
Dyslexia and the Brain
Spoken
language, recognizing faces, or identifying objects does not pose much of a
problem for the average person, even those with dyslexia. Dyslexia does seem to
impact reading, however. Researchers was once thought that dyslexia was
primarily a problem of language processing. As you may know, plasticity
is a term for the brain’s remarkable ability to rewire itself throughout a
person's life—a valuable cognitive asset. A study reported in the journal Neurons, used MRI to study the brains of
individuals with and without dyslexia. Their conclusion was that dyslexia may
represent a problem with the brain rewiring itself. That is, those who exhibit
struggles aligned with dyslexia appear to exhibit less brain plasticity as
compared with individuals without dyslexia issues. It is estimated that
dyslexia impacts one in ten persons worldwide, and up to seventeen percent of American schoolchildren. More
tomorrow.
Friday, February 3, 2017
Enmeshed Parenting, Five
Make no mistake, enmeshed
and unwise parenting can occur with mothers or fathers and with daughters as well
as sons. If the child grows up mental and emotionally and tries to separate
from the stifling parental relationship, although the child may like some of
the perks enmeshment gives them, the parent may try some strategy to retain the
enmeshed relationship. Either parent can ‘live vicariously’ through a child,
especially if their child (even in adulthood) is doing something the parent
would have liked to do. Once the parent clearly understands that his or her
behaviors are designed to obtain personal rewards and have nothing to do with
healthy functional parenting, there is a chance the parent may get some good
counseling and decide to get on with their own life. If the parent learns new
strategies and begins the process of disconnecting from the enmeshed relationships,
suggesting strategies for the child to use to learn to become a more resilient
and self-sufficient adult, the child may begin to act out: at age 25, or 35, or
45 or… Some adult children even threaten suicide if the parent begins to
disentangle from the enmeshment or shows signs of wanting to develop close friendships
with adults of their own age.
Thursday, February 2, 2017
Enmeshed Parenting, Four
Parents
often mistakenly believe that they are helping their child by rushing to meet
their emotional, mental, social, and financial needs. Truth is in many cases,
however, they are actually handicapping the child and preventing them from
having a rewarding, interdependent, and fulfilling adult life. If it is a
single parent who is enmeshed with a child, the parent may never marry or
remarry because the child becomes upset at just the thought of losing his or
her position as the adored offspring. If and when the child marries, the child
may choose a child-parent type of relationship. There is rarely a husband,
however, who wants to pamper his wife the way her father or mother does. The
first time the spouse does something that the adult child doesn’t like, the boy
will run home to Mommy so she can comfort him and solve his problem; the girl
will run home to Daddy so he can comfort her and solve her problem. If the
girl’s husband is happy to have a ‘child bride’ mentally and emotionally and willing
to have his father-in-law do all the nurturing, being content for his wife to
be’ arm candy,’ so be it. If not, the marriage will likely not last or the
couple will live very separate lives. The wife may interact physically and
sexually with her husband, emotionally she belongs to Daddy. More tomorrow.
Wednesday, February 1, 2017
Enmeshed Parenting, Three
The woman asked me, “What does my husband get
out of it, this preoccupation with trying to meet his adult daughter's every request?” Likely he feels powerful, important, and needed, and perhaps even
guilty for remarrying because he went against his daughter’s wishes. It’s a pretty
dreadful state of affairs. Her counselor had suggested that she sit down and
calmly explain what she perceives and tell her husband that this relationship
is not working for her. It’s possible he may be willing to see a good counselor
with her. However, if her husband gets his rewards from being an at-your-beck-and-call
Daddy and almost a surrogate husband to his 25-year-old daughter (hopefully
without any improper physical or emotional activity), basking in the child’s adoration,
there’s not much the wife can do. In that case, she may need to work with a good
counselor yourself to help you extract yourself from a very difficult
situation. In a sense he is ‘addicted’ to his daughter. His brain may even be
addicted to the adrenalin and dopamine that is produced in response to his
daughter’s frequent problems. As adrenalin increases, so does dopamine, which
gives Daddy two hits. He gets energy from the adrenalin and also feels better
as she tells him that he is her hero and she doesn’t know how she would live
without him. He needs to be ready for adult-level tantrums and manipulation, however,
should he learn to back off and let her grow up. More tomorrow.
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)